Âé¶¹ÒùÔº

April 29, 2025

Science explains how children best learn math—and yes, timed practice helps

Credit: Pixabay/CC0 Public Domain
× close
Credit: Pixabay/CC0 Public Domain

What's the best way for children to learn arithmetic—memorizing number values and multiplication tables, or studying math at a deeper, conceptual level?

Educators have long debated the merits of these two approaches, but a in Psychological Science in the Public Interest shows that children learn most effectively when instruction follows an evidence‑based cycle: grounding facts in conceptual understanding, using brief timed practice to make those facts automatic, and then returning to discussion and reflection to deepen that knowledge.

The report, titled "What the Science of Learning Teaches Us About Arithmetic Fluency," covers insights from developmental cognitive science to show how best to foster children's arithmetic fluency—generally defined as a child's ability to quickly and accurately solve problems. The researchers propose an expanded definition of arithmetic fluency to include not only the automatic recall of facts, such as 6 × 8 = 48, but also noticing and using number relationships to solve problems.

The authors of the paper include Nicole McNeil (University of Notre Dame), Nancy Jordan (University of Delaware), Alexandria Viegut (University of Wisconsin–Eau Claire), and Daniel Ansari (Western University). The researchers—all experts on children's math learning—describe findings on how arithmetic fluency develops, why it matters, and how educators can help students achieve it.

"We want to be clear: Educators don't have to choose between timed practice and rich classroom discussions," said lead author Nicole McNeil. "A carefully structured approach—pairing brief, timed sessions that strengthen facts in memory with purposeful reasoning and discussion activities that weave those facts into an integrated knowledge network—gives students the fluency they need to succeed."

Get free science updates with Science X Daily and Weekly Newsletters — to customize your preferences!

The paper incorporates findings from behavioral experiments, , neuroimaging, and design-based research to highlight the building blocks and consequences of fluency, which begins with number sense and numerical reasoning in preschool years. Much like phonics supports reading, early math experiences help children understand the meaning of numbers, relations, and operations. Parents and can use opportunities to help children count and label the total number of objects in everyday sets, such as blocks or cereal bits, the authors suggest.

To explain how fluency develops, the authors turn to foundational theories in that frame arithmetic learning not as a linear path, but as intertwined shifts between implicit and explicit knowledge. Children first begin with intuitive insights; they combine numbers without being able to communicate their reasoning (implicit knowledge). Gradually, they make that knowledge explicit, articulating patterns and strategies (e.g., "start with the bigger number," "order doesn't matter in addition") and justifying why they work.

Deliberate, well-structured practice then re-proceduralizes this explicit knowledge, allowing students to retrieve facts and strategies automatically, with little mental effort.

Instruction should nurture shifts in both directions between implicit and explicit knowledge, the authors say, drawing out the reasoning behind early intuitions and providing enough targeted practice for those ideas to become fast and effortless for higher‑level problem solving. Early arithmetic instruction, they assert, should include the following:

The authors also discuss the important role math fluency plays in children's futures. Studies show that students with strong fluency are better equipped to learn algebra, solve word problems, and reason with fractions. Research also links fluency with later-life outcomes like educational attainment and income.

The researchers call for evidence-based instructional practices that reflect what we know about how children learn. They advocate more developmental cognitive science in teacher preparation programs, particularly in , so that teachers can evaluate instructional strategies through the lens of learning science.

In an accompanying commentary, developmental psychologist Melissa E. Libertus (University of Pittsburgh) said the review raises a few factors for future research, such as the use of digital tools to facilitate personal arithmetic learning and the role of parents' math anxiety in their children's math skills.

Nevertheless, McNeil and her co-authors provide a convincing argument, Libertus said, because instruction plays an important role in helping students develop math skills to succeed in tomorrow's workforce.

More information: Nicole M. McNeil et al, What the Science of Learning Teaches Us About Arithmetic Fluency, Psychological Science in the Public Interest (2025).

Journal information: Psychological Science in the Public Interest

Load comments (0)

This article has been reviewed according to Science X's and . have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked
peer-reviewed publication
trusted source
proofread

Get Instant Summarized Text (GIST)

Children achieve arithmetic fluency most effectively through an instructional cycle that integrates conceptual understanding, brief timed practice to promote automatic recall, and reflective discussion. This approach supports both rapid fact retrieval and flexible problem-solving, with fluency linked to later success in advanced math and broader educational outcomes.

This summary was automatically generated using LLM.