Âé¶¹ÒùÔº

October 23, 2024

Experiments find people assume unidentified bystanders in a war zone are combatants, acceptable collateral damage

Percent of "Yes" responses to "Should the pilot fire?" depending on bystander identity, Study 1a. Error bars reflect 95% CIs. Asterisks denote Bonferroni corrected p-values compared with the unidentified bystander condition (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001). More innocent and uninvolved bystanders see a reduction in firing compared to bystanders who were guilty (e.g., ISIS solider) or tied to ISIS (e.g., Extremist Muslim, civilian war profiteer). This pattern does not hold for the unidentified bystander condition, for which most (58.5%) participants said the pilot should fire. Credit: Danielson et al., 2024, PLOS ONE, CC-BY 4.0 (creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)
× close
Percent of "Yes" responses to "Should the pilot fire?" depending on bystander identity, Study 1a. Error bars reflect 95% CIs. Asterisks denote Bonferroni corrected p-values compared with the unidentified bystander condition (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001). More innocent and uninvolved bystanders see a reduction in firing compared to bystanders who were guilty (e.g., ISIS solider) or tied to ISIS (e.g., Extremist Muslim, civilian war profiteer). This pattern does not hold for the unidentified bystander condition, for which most (58.5%) participants said the pilot should fire. Credit: Danielson et al., 2024, PLOS ONE, CC-BY 4.0 (creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)

People's bias toward sacrificing unknown bystanders appears to stem from assuming the unidentified person is an enemy, according to a study published October 23, 2024, in the open-access journal PLOS ONE by Scott Danielson from the University of Canterbury, New Zealand, and colleagues.

About as many civilians as soldiers die in war each year, some during strikes targeted at enemy combatants. There have been many reported cases of mistaking innocent civilians for enemy combatants, with the possibility of many more being unreported.

Danielson and colleagues conducted five experiments to test when people assume unknown bystanders in a combat zone are enemies rather than civilians, reducing their concerns about collateral damage. A total of 2,204 participants were presented with a realistic moral dilemma: A military pilot must decide whether to bomb a dangerous enemy target, also killing a .

The first two experiments used as the scenario a conflict between the U.S. and ISIS in Iraq. Here, few people endorsed bombing when the bystander was known to be an innocent civilian. However, when the bystander's identity was unknown, more than twice as many people endorsed the bombing.

Crucially, approximately half of American participants endorsed sacrificing unidentified bystanders despite no evidence they were enemies—a sacrificial rate higher than any identified target except a known enemy combatant. The subsequent three experiments used a fictional war setting and revealed a similar pattern of judgment was seen here too.

Bombing endorsement was predicted by attitudes toward total war: the theory that there should be no distinction between military and civilian targets in wartime conflict. The authors also noted key differences between the participant samples based on their country—bombing endorsement was lower for participants in the U.K. compared to the U.S., which the researchers attribute to national and in total war attitudes.

Get free science updates with Science X Daily and Weekly Newsletters — to customize your preferences!

According to the authors, these findings have implications for military strategists who must decide whether to attack areas with enemy militants and unidentified bystanders. The results support a common tendency in people to assume the bystanders are enemies, with important consequences if they turn out to be innocent civilians.

The real-world cases of civilians struck by bombs could result from the same error in judgment reported in this study. To minimize , future research should investigate how to reduce this bias and convince to evaluate more carefully who their weapons are targeting.

The authors add, "Fewer than one in four Americans support a military strike that kills a civilian, but 53% endorse a strike if the bystander is 'unidentified.' Data from five studies show that people are more likely to assume unknown bystanders are enemy combatants than civilians, which might explain the high civilian death toll in modern warfare."

More information: What I don't know can hurt you: Collateral combat damage seems more acceptable when bystander victims are unidentified, PLoS ONE (2024).

Journal information: PLoS ONE

Load comments (0)

This article has been reviewed according to Science X's and . have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked
peer-reviewed publication
trusted source
proofread

Get Instant Summarized Text (GIST)

This summary was automatically generated using LLM.