Âé¶¹ÒùÔº


What chess players can teach us about intelligence and expertise

What chess players can teach us about intelligence and expertise
British Chess Championship. Credit: Pat Baker from Gloucester, England/wikipedia, CC BY-SA

Are experts more intelligent than non-experts or do they just work harder? And why do some people reach high levels of expertise, while others just remain amateurs? These are some of the questions that cognitive scientists have tried to answer for more than a century. Now on chess players has started untangling the problem.

" Malcolm Gladwell suggests that 10,000 hours of practice will achieve expertise in virtually any skill. Rocky, in the famous series of boxing film montages, spent hours and hours of training to overcome fearsome adversaries and eventually succeed.

, however, are convinced that the amount of practice alone cannot account for individual differences in expert performance. They believe superior cognitive ability such as general intelligence or memory is fundamental to achieve mastery in one's field of expertise.

To evaluate the role of cognitive ability in expert performance, we recently carried out two reviews of the scientific literature about the role of cognitive ability in the acquisition of chess skill. We chose chess because it is one of the few fields that has a quantitative and reliable measure of skill (). It is, therefore, an ideal environment for studying expert performance and skill acquisition.

In fact, the study of chess players' memory and perception has contributed to our understanding of expertise in many other fields, such as music and computer programming. , the impact of chess on is comparable to that of Drosophila (fruit fly) for the field of genetics.

Clear results

We tried to answer two simple questions about chess. First, is intensive training all you need to become a chess master? Or do you need to be smarter than the average person to get there? Second, are chess players more intelligent than people not playing the game? The answers are important beyond chess and address fundamental questions in psychology and education.

, published in Intelligence, spans 19 studies and more than 1,700 participants. For this paper we searched for all the results concerned with chess players' performance on cognitive tasks.

We calculated the overall correlations between chess skill and four cognitive abilities: fluid intelligence (the ability of solving new problems and adapting to novel situations); processing speed (for example reaction time); short-term memory; and comprehension knowledge (knowledge and skills assimilated through experience, such as vocabulary and reading comprehension).

The results showed that chess skill correlated significantly with all the measures of cognitive ability. Put simply; the "smarter" the player, the higher the level of chess skill.

(including nearly 500 participants), also published in Intelligence, we included all the studies comparing chess players' and non-chess players' ability to solve . Then, we calculated the overall difference between chess players and non-. Chess players' performance was significantly superior in such as processing speed, planning, fluid intelligence and memory than in study participants who didn't play chess.

Nature versus nurture?

Our findings support the hypothesis that cognitive ability is a crucial element for the acquisition of chess skill. Of course, we have to be careful when establishing the direction of causality. It could be that intelligent people are more attracted to intellectual activities such as chess compared to the general population, or that they learn quicker.

But it could also be the case that practising cognitively demanding tasks makes people smarter. However, the latter possibility seems more unlikely, as recent research has found no causal relationship between and cognitive ability. Interestingly, the same lack of relationship has been found for .

While practice remains a necessary component of success in chess and other fields, it is just not sufficient to get to the top. If individuals with superior cognitive ability have better chances to achieve mastery in , it is likely that the same applies to other domains such as , , and .

Practice helps us to improve, but our improvements are strictly bounded to our cognitive ability. Sadly, good will is not enough.

Provided by The Conversation

This article was originally published on . Read the .The Conversation

Citation: What chess players can teach us about intelligence and expertise (2017, February 21) retrieved 16 June 2025 from /news/2017-02-chess-players-intelligence-expertise.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further


123 shares

Feedback to editors